student research

Hello! Are you wondering, "What is applied math research, anyway?" This page describes recent student projects related to my long-term program to understand collective behavior in locusts. Check these out to get a sense of what doing research with me might be like, and shoot me an email if you have any questions.

Summer 2024

I'm currently looking for student researchers to join my group for this summer! I hope to work with 2 or more students this summer on some aspect of my locust modeling work. Your summer research could be related to any aspect of my locust project, from modeling with differential equations to statistical inference and data science. You can see examples of past projects on this webpage.

If you are a Hamline student, we could apply to the Summer Collaborative Undergraduate Research (SCUR) program. The deadline for proposals is in the spring semester, but it's never too early to be in touch if you're interested!

Past Projects

Overview

Locusts form swarms with distinctive geometries that appear to aid in foraging. Fig A shows locusts moving perpendicular to the line of advancing insects through a lush agricultural field. In contrast, Fig C shows locusts moving parallel to the collective stream towards an isolated patch of vegetation. No leader directs the swarm to aggregate or move in these ways, instead both collective behaviors can be attributed to the interaction of rules that dictate an individual locust's attraction to food and social attraction/repulsion from other locusts. Understanding that interaction may eventually help identify efficient strategies for controlling locust outbreaks.

Most of the recent projects below involve data science in an effort to advance our empirical understanding of locust behavior within a swarm.

A Locusts travel in a planar front moving to the right as insects forage, leaving behind destroyed crops (brown) with uneaten crops ahead (green), from ABC. B Traveling pulse solution to our agent-based model of the the behavior observed in A. C Locusts forming a columnar stream over bare ground, from Wiki, in contrast with perpendicular movement in A. D Simulation of a first 2D agent-based model shows similar columnar structures to C. E Complex band shapes, with both clear fronts and extending columns, from AUS Dept of Agriculture. A NetLogo simulation of foraging locusts; the color scale indicates food density and the red points are foraging locusts.

Motion Tracking Individual Locusts in a Swarm

The form, speed, and density of these swarms is in large part due to social interactions, such as a locust orienting itself to move in the same direction as its neighbors. In order to deduce these interactions, we extracted numerical trajectories from raw video footage of locusts marching in the field. The video was provided by our collaborator Jerome Buhl (University of Adelaide), an eminent locust biologist. We conducted exploratory analysis of these trajectories, which continued in additional projects below.

On the left is a visual representation of our tracking process. Top: Locusts (dark dots) walk and hop in a naturally occurring swarm. Bottom: Processed video shows locusts (bright spots) detected by the algorithm (purple circles) and linked into trajectories (rainbow lines).

This project was part of the HMC Data Science REU, supported by NSF Grant DMS - 1757952.

Classifying Motion State with Support Vector Machines

Starting with the trajectories we collected from video, we investigated the movement behavior of individuals within the swarm. We developed a support vector machine (SVM) to classify motion into three states: stationary, walking, and hopping. The SVM used motion characteristics of the trajectories such as mean speed averaged over the near future and near past and standard deviation of speed over the same time window. We trained the SVM with data from diligently collected manual classification. Through cross-validation we estimated that the SVM was between 85-90% accurate. Approximately half the data was classified as hopping and the other half was split almost evenly between stationary and walking. This provided the first quantitative insight into how locusts move in a natural swarm!

The figure on the right shows an iteration of our SVM that shows clear clustering of the data. Figure credit to our collaborator Michael Culshaw-Maurer.

Deducing Insect Interactions from Field Data

We set out to infer how individuals interacted with their nearby neighbors. Our aim was to deduce biologically realistic rules for these interactions that could inform the creation of a future agent-based model that can produce swarms comparable to those observed in nature.

On the right, we plotted two-dimensional histograms of the position of nearby neighbors relative to a focal locust (red arrow) that is stopped (top) and crawling (bottom). Yellow indicates higher counts of neighbors while blue indicates lower counts. Note that crawling locusts have fewer neighbors in front of them. This suggested to us that when a crawling locust sees a neighbor directly in front, it either stops moving or turns to avoid a collision.

Figure credit goes to Jacob Landsberg (Haverford College '21) who conducted the bulk of this work as part of his senior thesis!

The Sociobiology of Foraging Strategies

Perhaps the most striking characteristic of locust behavior is that they manifest an epigenetic phase change where they transition from solitary individuals (when resources are plentiful) to gregarious foragers (when resources are sparse). It is believed that this behavioral transition evolved independently in multiple locust species on at least three continents. 

Our hypothesis is that this behavioral transition increases foraging efficiency for the swarm and survival potential for the species. We formulated this as a question in game theory and compared predictions using an extension of the agent-based model developed by Hannah Larson (HMC 2020) shown in the Overview Fig F. Dominant ecological theories of foraging describe the optimal search strategy of an individual only. Whereas game theory provides routes for optimizing strategies for groups. Our goal was to explain mathematically why locust behavior is bimodal.